Skip to main content
Advertisement
  • Loading metrics

Cryptochrome Mediates Light-Dependent Magnetosensitivity of Drosophila's Circadian Clock

Abstract

Since 1960, magnetic fields have been discussed as Zeitgebers for circadian clocks, but the mechanism by which clocks perceive and process magnetic information has remained unknown. Recently, the radical-pair model involving light-activated photoreceptors as magnetic field sensors has gained considerable support, and the blue-light photoreceptor cryptochrome (CRY) has been proposed as a suitable molecule to mediate such magnetosensitivity. Since CRY is expressed in the circadian clock neurons and acts as a critical photoreceptor of Drosophila's clock, we aimed to test the role of CRY in magnetosensitivity of the circadian clock. In response to light, CRY causes slowing of the clock, ultimately leading to arrhythmic behavior. We expected that in the presence of applied magnetic fields, the impact of CRY on clock rhythmicity should be altered. Furthermore, according to the radical-pair hypothesis this response should be dependent on wavelength and on the field strength applied. We tested the effect of applied static magnetic fields on the circadian clock and found that flies exposed to these fields indeed showed enhanced slowing of clock rhythms. This effect was maximal at 300 μT, and reduced at both higher and lower field strengths. Clock response to magnetic fields was present in blue light, but absent under red-light illumination, which does not activate CRY. Furthermore, cryb and cryOUT mutants did not show any response, and flies overexpressing CRY in the clock neurons exhibited an enhanced response to the field. We conclude that Drosophila's circadian clock is sensitive to magnetic fields and that this sensitivity depends on light activation of CRY and on the applied field strength, consistent with the radical pair mechanism. CRY is widespread throughout biological systems and has been suggested as receptor for magnetic compass orientation in migratory birds. The present data establish the circadian clock of Drosophila as a model system for CRY-dependent magnetic sensitivity. Furthermore, given that CRY occurs in multiple tissues of Drosophila, including those potentially implicated in fly orientation, future studies may yield insights that could be applicable to the magnetic compass of migratory birds and even to potential magnetic field effects in humans.

Author Summary

Magnetic fields influence endogenous clocks controlling the sleep–wake cycle of animals, but the underyling mechanisms are unclear. Birds that can do magnetic compass orientation also depend on light, and the blue-light photopigment cryptochrome was proposed to act as a navigational magnetosensor. Here we tested the role of cryptochrome as a light-dependent magnetosensor of the clock in the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster. In wild-type flies we found that constant magnetic fields slowed down the speed of the clock in a dose-dependent manner—but only in the presence of blue light. In mutants lacking functional cryptochrome, the magnetic fields had no significant effects on the endogenous clock, whereas the effects were enhanced after overexpression of cryptochrome. Our data suggest that cryptochrome works as a magnetosensor in the endogenous clock when it is excited by blue light. Our work supports previous data showing that fruit flies need functional cryptochrome to perceive a magnetic field, demonstrating that the interaction of cryptochome and magnetic fields are not just for the birds.

Introduction

Endogenous clocks help organisms to adapt to the 24-h cycle of the earth. One of the main characteristics of endogenous clocks is that they continue to oscillate even in the absence of external time cues. Under such conditions, they “free-run” with their endogenous inherited periods that are slightly different from 24 h. Therefore, they are also called circadian clocks. Under natural conditions circadian clocks are synchronized to the 24-h cycle on Earth by different Zeitgebers. The most important Zeitgeber is the light–dark cycle, but temperature, humidity, and the social environment can also serve as Zeitgebers. Additionally, magnetic fields have been discussed as agents that synchronize circadian clocks since 1960, when Brown et al. [1] found that the circadian rhythms of fiddler crabs and other organisms are influenced by small changes in the intensity of Earth's magnetic field. In particular, the electromagnetic field with a frequency of 10 Hz is known to show a prominent 24-h oscillation [2]; thus, the field could serve as geophysical synchronizer of the circadian clock. Although electromagnetic fields are not consciously perceived by humans, Wever [3] demonstrated that the circadian activity rhythm of humans can be entrained by an artificial 10 Hz electromagnetic field, mimicking the natural oscillations on Earth. The activity of mice, house flies, and fruit flies could also be synchronized or phase-shifted by 10 Hz electric fields applied for 12 h daily [4,5]. Cyclically changing intensities of a static magnetic field can also work as weak synchronizers of the circadian clock, as shown by Bliss and Heppner [6] for house sparrows. The latter study was motivated by the idea that birds perform magnetic compass orientation and that this ability depends on a functional clock. Thus, the authors speculated about a link between the magnetosense and the circadian clock.

Despite the compelling effects of (electro)-magnetic fields on the circadian clock, the mechanisms by which the magnetic field is perceived and how it manipulates circadian clocks are unknown. Two main models of magnetic field sensing are currently under discussion: (1) the magnetite model proposing a primary process involving tiny crystals of permanently magnetic material [7], and (2) the radical-pair model suggesting a “chemical compass” based on singlet–triplet transitions in photopigments [810].

The radical-pair mechanism, in which the magnetosensor is a photopigment, was first suggested by Schulten and coworkers [810]. The first step of the reaction is the absorption of a photon of light energy by the pigment molecule, leading to the transient formation of radical pairs (unpaired electrons) with opposite electron spin states (singlet electrons). The model predicts that these electrons are close enough together that the unpaired electron spin states can undergo transition, at some frequency, from antiparallel to parallel spin states (singlet–triplet intersystem crossing). Such singlet–triplet intersystem crossing is a natural feature of many photochemical reactions that, in fact, may have nothing to do with magnetosensitivity and is the result of the system reaching the most energetically favorable state subsequent to absorption of photon energy. The critical feature of a magnetosensitive radical-pair reaction is that the radicals have a sufficiently long lifetime and are oriented in such a way that singlet–triplet transitions are modified by their alignment in the presence of an applied magnetic field. In such a case, product yield or rate of formation would be different depending on the relative amount of singlet or triplet formation, and these changes in output would provide a means for the organisms to detect the effects of even very weak applied fields. This presumed radical-pair mechanism has indeed been observed experimentally in reactions involving organic compounds [11] and in biological systems such as photosynthetic reaction centers [12,13].

Since the radical-pair mechanism of magnetosensitivity depends on the activation of the relevant photopigment by light, it is wavelength dependent. Magnetosensitivity is maximal at the absorbance maximum of the photopigment, and it depends on the strength of the magnetic field. Ritz et al. [10] calculated the yield of the triplet products in relation to the strength of the magnetic field and the angle of the field with respect to the alignment of the radical pair in space. They found that there is a small window of the magnetic field strength at which the radical-pair mechanism furnishes magnetosensory capacities. In agreement with these calculations, Wiltschko and Wiltschko found that the magnet compass orientation of birds works only at a certain range of magnetic intensities [14].

That magnetoperception is wavelength dependent has been shown not only in birds but also in newts and fruit flies. As photopigments, cryptochromes (CRYs) have been proposed [9,15,16]. CRYs are UV- and blue-absorbing photoreceptors, found in plants and animals, that contain flavin adenine dinucleotide (FAD) as chromophore [17] and have been shown to form radical pairs subsequent to light activation [18]. CRYs are expressed in the eyes of mammals [19] and migratory birds [20,21], where crucial magnetoreceptors have been localized [22,23]. However, CRYs are also expressed in the circadian clock neurons of mice and flies [2427]. This raises the possibility that magnetic fields can interact directly with the circadian clock.

The fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster is best suited to reveal a putative role of CRYs in the magnetosensitivity of the circadian clock, because there is only one CRY, and because mutants are available that either knock out the binding domain of the FAD (cryb mutants, [28]) or the entire CRY molecule (cry0 mutants, [29]; cryOUT mutants, [25]; cryΔ mutants, [30]). The molecular mechanisms of the circadian clock are largely unraveled. The main players of the clock are the clock genes period, timeless, Clock, and cycle, which participate in complex molecular feedback loops to generate the circadian oscillation [31,32]. CRY is known to play an important role in the photoreception pathway to Drosophila's endogenous clock [27]. Light promotes the binding of CRY to TIMELESS (TIM), leading to the degradation of TIM [33,34]. Under light–dark cycles the action of CRY leads to a “reset” of the clock every morning. However, under constant light (LL), TIM-mediated degradation by CRY occurs continuously. Therefore, the amount of TIM is reduced and the clock slows down (i.e., the free-running period of the locomotor rhythm is lengthened). When the constant illumination exceeds a critical intensity (> 10 lx), TIM disappears completely and the clock stops running (the flies become arrhythmic [35]).

On the basis of the background described above we speculated that a magnetic field might influence the circadian clock via CRY under blue light but not red light conditions. To investigate this possibility, we measured the free-running periods of the flies' locomotor rhythms before and during exposure to a magnetic field under constant weak blue light or red light illumination. The blue and red lights were adjusted to such low intensities that in the absence of the magnetic field the flies showed only a slightly longer period than when they were under constant dark (DD) conditions. We found that 40% of the flies further lengthened the free-running periods during the exposure to a magnetic field of 300 μT under blue light illumination. This effect was completely absent under red light conditions. We also demonstrate that the response of fruit flies to the magnetic field is not only wavelength dependent, but also dependent on the strength of the magnetic field, as is implied by the radical-pair model. Furthermore, the magnetic field could not induce any period lengthening in cryb and cryOUT mutant flies; but period lengthening was enhanced after overexpressing CRY in the clock neurons. Our results indicate that the magnetic field influences the circadian clock via CRY.

Results

Finding the Right Settings

The illumination of the flies was a critical parameter during our experiments, because we intended to activate CRY, but not to such an extent that arrhythmic behavior would occur. We recorded the flies under different intensities of blue light (465–470 nm) and found that 0.18 μW/cm2 was a suitable light intensity for the experiments, at which the flies did not become arrhythmic, but showed a significant period lengthening. The mean period at 0.18 μW/cm2 blue light was 25.8 ± 0.14 h (mean ± standard error of the mean [SEM], n = 25). This period was 1.7 h longer than under DD conditions [36], indicating that CRY was activated by the constant weak blue light and that it provoked a constant weak TIM degradation. Our rationale was that we should see further changes in the free-running period of the flies as soon as they were exposed to the magnetic field affecting the singlet–triplet intersystem crossing of FAD in light-activated CRY.

Effects of Magnetic Fields on the Free-Running Period

In a first experiment we tested the influence of magnetic fields of different intensities (Figure 1). The radical-pair mechanism predicts that there should be an optimal magnetic strength and that the effects would be smaller under too low or too high magnetic fields [9]. To enable comparison with previous experiments [37] we chose static magnetic fields of 0 μT, 150 μT, 300 μT, and 500 μT; excepting the control of 0 μT these are, respectively, 3, 6, and 10 times stronger than natural magnetic fields. The free-running periods of the flies were determined before and during the application of the constant magnetic fields and the changes in period were calculated (independent of their direction). We found that even flies without exposure to a magnetic field exhibited period changes over the recording time, but that these changes were significantly smaller than those occurring under the influence of a magnetic field of 300 μT (Figures 2A, 2B and 3A). ANOVA revealed that the period changes depended significantly on the strength of the magnetic field (Figure 3A). Most flies lengthened their periods in response to the magnetic field (Figure 2A); but there are also some flies with shortened periods (Figure 2B). To quantify the number of flies with shortened and lengthened periods we defined the following categories (Table 1): Flies exhibiting period changes smaller than 0.5 h (as most wild-type flies did) were defined as flies showing no effects. Flies with shortened or lengthened periods by 0.5 h or more were defined as flies showing period shortening or lengthening, respectively. The third category consisted of flies in which periodogram analysis could not detect any significant period. These were defined as arrhythmic. χ2 analysis revealed that the number of flies with lengthened periods was significantly higher when a magnetic field of 300 μT was applied (Table 1).

thumbnail
Figure 1. Schematic Representation of the Experimental Apparatus

The locomotor recording device was described in detail previously [59,60]. Here eight of 32 spectrophotometer cuvettes are shown as representative “houses” for flies. Two monochromatic LEDs were placed in front of each cuvette, and the flies were illuminated homogenously in the cuvettes. Helmholtz coils were placed above and below the recording device, generating a static magnetic field (dotted light with arrows) perpendicular to the plane of the recording device. The infrared sensors for monitoring the activity of the flies were on the ends of the cuvettes opposite the LED illumination.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1000086.g001

thumbnail
Figure 2. Representative Free-Running Locomotor Rhythms before and during the Exposure to a Magnetic Field

Actograms of two wild-type flies (A and B), one cryb mutant (C) and one CRY-overexpressing fly (tim-gal4;uas-cry) (D) are depicted. All flies were exposed to blue light of 0.18 μW/cm2 throughout the experiment and to a magnetic field of 300 μT during the second half of the experiment (indicated by gray areas). The free-running periods of 40% of wild-type flies lengthened (A) and 12% shortened (B) during exposure to the magnetic field. These effects were not observed in the majority of cryb mutant flies (C). More than 60% of CRY-overexpressing flies exhibited arrhythmicity after the magnetic field (D).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1000086.g002

thumbnail
Figure 3. Period Changes (+ SEM) Observed in the Free-Running Rhythm of Wild-Type Flies after Application of Magnetic Fields under Blue Light and Red Light Illumination

Under blue light (A), magnetic fields of different strength were applied, and ANOVA revealed that the observed period alterations significantly depended on the strength of the magnetic field (F3,101 = 3.0; p = 0.03). A post-hoc test revealed that the period change at 300 μT was significantly different from that at 0 μT. Under red light (B), the period alterations produced by a magnetic field of 300 μT were not significantly different from those occurring spontaneously (without a magnetic field). Numbers in the columns indicate the number of included flies. Flies that became arrhythmic are excluded from this analysis (compare Table 1).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1000086.g003

thumbnail
Table 1.

Percentages of flies That Show Alterations in Rhythmic Behaviour after Application of a Magnetic Field

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1000086.t001

Next we tested whether the effect of the magnetic field depends on the wavelength of the constant light. For that purpose the experiments were performed under a constant red light (625–630 nm) of 0.18 μW/cm2, with or without applying a field of 300 μT. We found no difference in period changes between the two groups (Figure 3B; Table 1). This result clearly indicates that the lengthening of the periods by the magnetic field is dependent on the wavelength.

Cryptochrome Is Involved in Magnetoreception

So far our results are consistent with the idea that magnetoreception involves a radical-pair mechanism occurring in a photopigment. In the next step we wanted to know whether CRY might be the relevant photopigment, so we tested cryb mutants in which FAD binding is impaired [38] and cryOUT mutants that lack CRY completely [25]. As expected, both mutants show short periods under blue light of 0.18 μW/cm2 (Figure 2C), because TIM is not degraded due to the mutation in the CRY protein (e.g., [34]). The free-running periods of cryb and cryOUT mutants were 23.4 ± 0.05 h (n = 25) and 23.7 ± 0.04 h (n = 26), respectively. After exposure to the 300 μT magnetic field, 88.0% of cryb flies and 76.9% of cryOUT flies did not show any period changes (Figure 2C; Table 1), and when changes occurred these were significantly smaller than those in wild-type flies (Figure 4). This indicates that CRY is involved in the magnetically sensitive response of the fly.

thumbnail
Figure 4. Period Changes (+ SEM) Observed in the Free-Running Rhythm of Wild-Type and Control Flies and Fly Strains with Manipulated CRY after Application of a Magnetic Field (300 μT) and Blue Light

In cryb mutants CRY cannot bind its chromophore flavin, cryOUT mutants are devoid of CRY, and tim-gal4;uas-cry flies have CRY overexpressed in all clock neurons. ANOVA revealed a significant influence of the strain on the period change (F3,76 = 29.88; p < 0.001). A consecutive post-hoc test revealed that cryb and cryOUT mutants exhibited significantly less period change than CRY-overexpressing flies, which showed significant larger period changes than all other strains. Numbers in or above the columns indicate the number of included flies. Since most tim-gal4;uas-cry flies became arrhythmic in the magnetic field (Table 1) only eight flies could be included in the analysis.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1000086.g004

Next we wanted to test whether we could not only diminish the responses to the magnetic field by knocking out CRY, but also increase the responses by overexpressing CRY in the clock neurons. This test could be done by expressing CRY under the control of the strong clock-gene promoter of the timeless gene (in timgal4/uas-cry flies). The circadian clock of such flies shows increased light sensitivity [38], and accordingly we found that CRY-overexpressing flies showed significantly longer free-running periods than wild-type flies under blue light. This result indicates that the higher amount of light-activated CRY degrades TIM to a greater extent. The average period of the 24 tested CRY-overexpressing flies was 26.9 ± 0.33 h, and one fly showed arrhythmicity. After exposure to the magnetic field, most CRY-overexpressing flies became arrhythmic (Figure 2D; Table 1) and the remaining ones showed large period changes (Figure 4). All flies except one lengthened their free-running periods (Table 1). These results show that the degree of responsiveness to the magnetic field is directly linked to the levels of CRY present in the fly, and that the site of action of the magnetic field effect can be directly localized to the clock neurons, in which CRY is active in setting the circadian clock.

Discussion

We show here that Drosophila's clock is magnetosensitive, and that this sensitivity depends on CRY. When the first studies were performed that showed magnetosensitivity of the circadian clock, CRY had not been detected yet and the mechanism of how the clock is influenced by magnetic fields was completely elusive. Now, we know that CRY is expressed in the clock neurons of Drosophila [25,30], that CRY absorbs UV and blue-green light [39], and that CRY consecutively binds to TIM and induces its degradation [34]. Under constant conditions this process leads to period lengthening or arrhythmicity [35]. The magnetic field effect that we observed in our study led to further lengthening of the period, in the same way as an increased intensity of blue light would lead to enhanced CRY function. The effect of a magnetic field was particularly pronounced in CRY-overexpressing flies, in which small differences in CRY function should be significantly amplified. Additionally, we could show that this reaction depends on the strength of the magnetic field and on the wavelength of the light. Red light that is outside of the absorption and action spectra of CRY [3942] did not provoke significant responses. Consistent with the involvement of cryptochrome, cryb and cryOUT mutants respond neither to blue-green light nor to the magnetic field.

Our results strongly support the radical-pair model suggesting light-activated flavin-based photoreceptors as sensors for magnetic fields. In several animals magnetoreception has been shown to depend preferentially on blue light [4347]. Ritz et al. [10] stated first that CRYs are suitable molecules for such photoreceptor-based magnetoreception. CRYs are blue-green photoreceptors in plants and flies [17,28,48,49] and have been shown to form radical pairs upon photoexcitation [18] as is true for the closely related DNA photolyases [50]. Other classes of photoreceptors, such as phototropins [51] and chlorophylls [52] found in plants, also undergo radical-pair reactions. However, rhodopsins, which are the major photosensory pigments in flies and other animals, are not able to form radical pairs because photoexcitation leads to cistrans isomerization of retinal rather than an electron transfer [53]. Thus, CRYs are the only currently known class of molecules found in animals that are likely to fulfill the physical and chemical characteristics that are required for functioning as the primary magnetic sensor.

The first evidence that CRYs may work as magnetosensors comes from the plant Arabidopsis thaliana, where it was shown that CRYs mediate magnetic field-dependent hypocotyl growth inhibition under blue light [37]. In these experiments, the effect of the magnetic field was to increase the plant's response to perceived blue light by enhancing cryptochrome activity, similarly to the presently observed magnetic field effect in flies. The second evidence comes from the fly D. melanogaster (that paper was published during the preparation of the present manuscript [54]). Gegear et al. [54] showed clearly that fruit flies need functional CRY to perceive a magnetic field that was paired with a sugar reward during a classical learning experiment. Although this study did not analyze the effect of magnetic field strength on CRY activity, these data are highly consistent with our present observations that the magnetic field intensifies the effects of blue light on the clock. The radical-pair mechanism predicts that the rate or yield of product formation from such a reaction will be altered by an applied magnetic field. Therefore, the pairing of blue light and the magnetic field may lead to the perception of more intense (brighter) light as compared to blue light of the same intensity presented alone. Consequently, hypocotyl growth was inhibited to a higher degree in Arabidopsis [37], and flies learned to correlate a more intense light signal under magnetic field presence with the sucrose award [54]. In our experiments the circadian clock lengthened its period or became arrhythmic as if it experienced blue light of a higher intensity. All three phenomena clearly do not depend on directional effects of the magnetic field.

That flies can also perform magnetic compass orientation was shown first by Wehner and Labhart [55] and further elaborated by Phillips and Sayeed [43] and Dommer et al. [47]. The latter two studies showed that Drosophila adult males and larvae are capable of learning the alignment of important environmental gradients (i.e., light, food, and humidity) with respect to the geomagnetic field [43,47]. These responses were maximal under UV light, but the involvement of CRY was not tested.

The ability of animals to detect geomagnetic fields has substantial biological importance, because it is used by invertebrate and vertebrate species for orientation and navigation [56]. Almost certainly CRY also plays a crucial role in this process (see [10,16,20,21,57] for discussion). Fruit flies may be well suited to further unraveling the involvement of CRY in the mechanisms of magnetic compass orientation. Since CRY is also present outside the clock neurons in tissues such as the compound eye and the ellipsoid body [25], in which it might show a directional alignment, it will be most rewarding to test the roles of these structures in magnetic compass orientation.

In conclusion, we provide the first evidence of a mechanistic link between CRY and magnetic sensitivity in the circadian clock of Drosophila. Our data provide powerful support for the radical-pair hypothesis, because they show the predicted relationship between strength of the applied field and the reaction (here period of the clock). Furthermore, the fact that the magnetic field leads to enhanced photosensitivity of CRY in both plants and flies argues for a similar mechanism of magnetoperception in all organisms that contain CRYs, including humans. Finally, given the recent observations that sun compass orientation in the monarch butterfly is dependent on the circadian clock and on CRY [57], this effect of magnetic field on the circadian clock may in fact play a role in fly orientation.

Materials and Methods

Fly strains.

CantonS was used as wild-type strain and compared with the cry mutants cryb [28] and cryOUT [25]. For CRY overexpression in all clock neurons, w;tim(UAS)gal4 flies [58] and w;UAS-cry flies [38] were crossed. All flies were reared under 12:12 light-dark cycles on Drosophila medium (0.8% agar, 2.2% sugar beet syrup, 8.0% malt extract, 1.8% yeast, 1.0% soy flour, 8.0% corn flour, and 0.3% hydroxybenzoic acid) at either 20 °C or 25 °C. Only adult male flies at an age of 3–6 d were used for the experiments.

Recording the locomotor activity of flies.

Locomotor activity of individual male flies was recorded automatically with infrared light beams at constant 20 °C as described previously [59,60]. Briefly, the flies were confined to photometer cuvettes that were placed with one end in an infrared light beam. The number of light beam crosses by a walking fly was monitored during consecutive 1-min intervals. Monochromatic LEDs (Lumitronix LED-Technik, Jungingen, Germany) emitting blue (465–470 nm) and red (625–630 nm) light were used as light sources to illuminate the flies. Light intensity of both LEDs was adjusted to 0.18 μW/cm2. Under constant blue light of 0.18 μW/cm2, flies displayed slightly longer free-running periods than those in DD, but did not become arrhythmic. To expose the flies to magnetic fields, Helmholtz coils of sufficient diameter (20 cm) to cover the locomotor recording device were placed above and below the recording device, generating a perpendicular static magnetic field (DC mode of current) (Figure 1). The magnetic field intensity was controlled by changing the voltage of the power supply that connects to the coils and was placed outside the recording device. The magnetic intensities were measured by a gaussmeter (475 DSP Gaussmeter, Lake Shore Cryotronics, Ohio, United States). To generate magnetic fields of 150, 300, and 500 μT, 6, 12, and 20 volts were given to the coils, respectively. For comparison, the natural magnetic field of the Earth ranges between 30 μT (equator) and 60 μT (poles). We used static magnetic fields of 150–500 μT instead of oscillating magnetic fields of natural strength to compare our results directly with former results on A. thaliana [37]. The free-running rhythms were first recorded for 10–15 d without a magnetic field, and then fields of four different intensities (0, 150, 300, 500 μT) were applied. All experiments were performed in the same recording device that was positioned in a climate chamber. The climate chamber was surrounded by a metal case shielding the flies from the local geomagnetic field. In the absence of the applied artificial magnetic fields the Gaussmeter could not detect any magnetic field strength in the chamber containing the flies. Furthermore, the climate chamber was tightly temperature controlled. Ventilators close to the Helmholtz coils immediately blew away any heated air. A maximum of 0.5 °C temperature fluctuations were measured close to the Helmholtz coils. Such small temperature changes are known not to influence the circadian clock.

Data analysis.

The raw data were displayed as actograms using the program El Temps (Antoni Diez-Noguera, Barcelona, 1999; http://www.el-temps.com). To evaluate the periods of the free-running rhythms, data for 10 d before and during the exposure to the magnetic field was chosen and the periods were calculated by the χ2 periodogram analysis [61]. This is an objective method making a double-blind experimental approach needless. In the control flies that did not experience any magnetic field, exactly the same subjective days of the flies were used for period determination. If a peak above the 0.05 confidence level appeared in the periodogram, the period was designated as statistically significant.

Statistics.

A one-way ANOVA was used to test for significant influences of the magnetic fields or of CRY on the amount of period changes. A subsequent post hoc test with Bonferroni's adjustment was applied for pairwise comparisons (Systat 11; SPSS, Chicago, Illinois, United States). χ2 analysis was used to test whether the number of arrhythmic flies or the number of flies showing period lengthening increased significantly upon exposure to the magnetic field. Values were regarded as significantly different at p < 0.05.

Acknowledgments

We thank Misako Yoshii for drawing Figure 1, Ralf Stanewsky for the cryb and cryOUT mutants, Michael Young for the tim-gal4 fly line, Patrick Emery for the uas-cry fly line, Wolfgang Engelmann for comments on the manuscript, Günther Stöckl for building the magnetic coil elements, and Angelika Kühn for excellent technical assistance.

Author Contributions

MA and CH-F conceived and designed the experiments. TY performed the experiments and analyzed the data. CH-F wrote the paper.

References

  1. 1. Brown FA (1960) Response to pervasive geophysical factors and the biological clock problem. Cold Spring Harbor Sym Quant Biol 25: 57–71.
  2. 2. König H (1959) Atmospherics niedriger Frequenzen. Z angew Physik 11: 264.
  3. 3. Wever R (1968) Einfluß schwacher elektro-magnetischer Felder auf die circadiane Periodik des Menschen. Naturwissenschaften 1: 29–32.
  4. 4. Engelmann W, Hellrung W, Johnsson A (1996) Circadian locomotor activity of Musca flies: Recording method and effects of 10Hz square-wave electric fields. Bioelectromagnetics 17: 100–110.
  5. 5. Dowse HB, Palmer JD (1969) Entrainment of circadian activity rhythms in mice by electrostatic fields. Nature 222: 564–566.
  6. 6. Bliss VL, Heppner FH (1976) Circadian activity rhythm influenced by near zero magnetic field. Nature 261: 411–412.
  7. 7. Kirschvink JL, Walker MM, Chang SB, Dizon AE, Peterson KA (1985) Chains of single-domain magnetite particles in chinook salmon, Oncorhynchus tshawytscha. J Comp Physiol A 157: 375–381.
  8. 8. Schulten K, Swenberg CE, Weller A (1978) A biomagnetic sensory mechanism based on magnetic field modulated coherent electron spin motion. Z Phys Chem NF111. pp. 1–5.
  9. 9. Schulten K, Windemuth A (1986) Model for a physiological magnetic compass. In: Maret G, Boccara N, Kiepenheuer J, editors. Biophysical Effects of Steady Magnetic Fields. Berlin, Heidelberg, New York: Springer Verlag. pp. 99–106. editors.
  10. 10. Ritz T, Adem S, Schulten K (2000) A model for photoreceptor-based magnetoreception in birds. Biophys J 78: 707–718.
  11. 11. Maeda K, Henbest KB, Cintolesi F, Kuprov I, Rodgers CT, et al. (2008) Chemical compass model for avian magnetoreception. Nature 453: 387–390.
  12. 12. Hoff AJ (1981) Magnetic-field effects on photosynthetic reactions. Q Rev Biophys 14: 599–665.
  13. 13. Boxer SG, Chidsey CED, Roelofs MG (1983) Magnetic-field effects on reaction yields in the solid state—an example form photosynthetic reaction centers. Ann Rev Phys Chem 34: 389–417.
  14. 14. Wiltschko W, Wiltschko R (1972) Magnetic compass orientation in European robins. Science 176: 62–64.
  15. 15. Ritz T, Dommer DH, Phillips JB (2002) Shedding light on vertebrate magnetoreception. Neuron 34: 503–506.
  16. 16. Rodgers CT, Hore PJ (2009) Chemical magnetoreception in birds: The radical pair mechanism. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106: 353–360.
  17. 17. Cashmore AR, Jarillo JA, Wu YJ, Liu D (1999) Cryptochromes: blue light receptors for plants and animals. Science 284: 760–765.
  18. 18. Giovani B, Byrdin M, Ahmad M, Brettel K (2003) Light-induced electron transfer in a cryptochrome blue-light photoreceptor. Nat Struct Biol 10: 489–490.
  19. 19. Miyamoto Y, Sancar A (1998) Vitamin B2-based blue-light photoreceptors in the retinohypothalamic tract as the photoactive pigments for setting the circadian clock in mammals. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 95: 6097–6102.
  20. 20. Möller A, Sagasser S, Wiltschko W, Schierwater B (2004) Retinal cryptochrome in a migratory passerine bird: a possible transducer for the avian magnetic compass. Naturwissenschaften 91: 585–588.
  21. 21. Mouritsen H, Janssen-Bienhold U, Liedvogel M, Feenders G, Stalleicken J, et al. (2004) Cryptochromes and neuronal-activity markers colocalize in the retina of migratory birds during magnetic orientation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 101: 14294–14299.
  22. 22. Wiltschko W, Traudt J, Gunturkun O, Prior H, Wiltschko R (2002) Lateralization of magnetic compass orientation in a migratory bird. Nature 419: 467–470.
  23. 23. Prior H, Wiltschko R, Stapput K, Gunturkun O, Wiltschko W (2004) Visual lateralization and homing in pigeons. Behav Brain Res 154: 301–310.
  24. 24. Maywood ES, O'Brien JA, Hastings MH (2003) Expression of mCLOCK and other circadian clock-relevant proteins in the mouse suprachiasmatic nuclei. J Neuroendocrinol 15: 329–334.
  25. 25. Yoshii T, Todo T, Wulbeck C, Stanewsky R, Helfrich-Förster C (2008) Cryptochrome is present in the compound eyes and a subset of Drosophila's clock neurons. J Comp Neurol 508: 952–966.
  26. 26. Hardin PE, Glossop NR (1999) Perspectives: neurobiology. The CRYs of flies and mice. Science 286: 2460–2461.
  27. 27. Hall JC (2000) Cryptochromes: sensory reception, transduction, and clock functions subserving circadian systems. Curr Opin Neurobiol 10: 456–466.
  28. 28. Stanewsky R, Kaneko M, Emery P, Beretta B, Wager-Smith K, et al. (1998) The cryb mutation identifies cryptochrome as a circadian photoreceptor in Drosophila. Cell 95: 681–692.
  29. 29. Dolezelova E, Dolezel D, Hall JC (2007) Rhythm defects caused by newly engineered null mutations in Drosophila's cryptochrome gene. Genetics 177: 329–345.
  30. 30. Benito J, Houl JH, Roman GW, Hardin PE (2008) The blue-light photoreceptor CRYPTOCHROME is expressed in a subset of circadian oscillator neurons in the Drosophila CNS. J Biol Rhythms 23: 296–307.
  31. 31. Zheng X, Sehgal A (2008) Probing the relative importance of molecular oscillations in the circadian clock. Genetics 178: 1147–1155.
  32. 32. Sheeba V, Kaneko M, Sharma VK, Holmes TC (2008) The Drosophila circadian pacemaker circuit: Pas De Deux or Tarantella. Crit Rev Biochem Mol Biol 43: 37–61.
  33. 33. Ceriani MF, Darlington TK, Staknis D, Mas P, Petti AA, et al. (1999) Light-dependent sequestration of TIMELESS by CRYPTOCHROME. Science 285: 553–556.
  34. 34. Busza A, Emery-Le M, Rosbash M, Emery P (2004) Roles of the two Drosophila CRYPTOCHROME structural domains in circadian photoreception. Science 304: 1503–1506.
  35. 35. Konopka RJ, Pittendrigh C, Orr D (1989) Reciprocal behaviour associated with altered homeostasis and photosensitivity of Drosophila clock mutants. J Neurogenet 6: 1–10.
  36. 36. Helfrich-Förster C (2000) Differential control of morning and evening components in the activity rhythm of Drosophila melanogaster—sex-specific differences suggest a different quality of activity. J Biol Rhythms 15: 135–154.
  37. 37. Ahmad M, Galland P, Ritz T, Wiltschko R, Wiltschko W (2007) Magnetic intensity affects cryptochrome-dependent responses in Arabidopsis thaliana. Planta 225: 615–624.
  38. 38. Emery P, So WV, Kaneko M, Hall JC, Rosbash M (1998) CRY, a Drosophila clock and light-regulated cryptochrome, is a major contributor to circadian rhythm resetting and photosensitivity. Cell 95: 669–679.
  39. 39. Berndt A, Kottke T, Breitkreuz H, Dvorsky R, Hennig S, et al. (2007) A novel photoreaction mechanism for the circadian blue light photoreceptor Drosophila cryptochrome. J Biol Chem 282: 13011–13021.
  40. 40. VanVickle-Chavez SJ, Van Gelder RN (2007) Action spectrum of Drosophila cryptochrome. J Biol Chem 282: 10561–10566.
  41. 41. Helfrich-Förster C, Edwards T, Yasuyama K, Wisotzki B, Schneuwly S, et al. (2002) The extraretinal eyelet of Drosophila: development, ultrastructure, and putative circadian function. J Neurosci 22: 9255–9266.
  42. 42. Hoang N, Schleicher E, Kacprzak S, Bouly JP, Picot M, et al. (2008) Human and Drosophila cryptochromes are light activated by flavin photoreduction in living cells. PLoS Biol 6: e160.
  43. 43. Phillips JB, Sayeed O (1993) Wavelength-dependent effects of light on magnetic compass orientation in Drosophila melanogaster. J Comp Physiol [A] 172: 303–308.
  44. 44. Wiltschko W, Munr U, Ford H, Wiltschko R (1993) Red light disrupts magnetic orientation of migratory birds. Nature 364: 525–527.
  45. 45. Wiltschko W, Wiltschko R (1995) Migratory orientation of European Robins is affected by the wavelength of light as well as by a magnetic pulse. J Comp Physiol A 177: 363–369.
  46. 46. Muheim R, Backman J, Akesson S (2002) Magnetic compass orientation in European robins is dependent on both wavelength and intensity of light. J Exp Biol 205: 3845–3856.
  47. 47. Dommer DH, Gazzolo PJ, Painter MS, Phillips JB (2008) Magnetic compass orientation by larval Drosophila melanogaster. J Insect Physiol 54: 719–726.
  48. 48. Ahmad M, Cashmore AR (1993) HY4 gene of Arabidopsis thaliana encodes a protein with characteristics of a blue-light photoreceptor. Nature 366: 162–166.
  49. 49. Sancar A (2000) Cryptochrome: the second photoactive pigment in the eye and its role in circadian photoreception. Annu Rev Biochem 69: 31–67.
  50. 50. Byrdin M, Sartor V, Eker AP, Vos MH, Aubert C, et al. (2004) Intraprotein electron transfer and proton dynamics during photoactivation of DNA photolyase from E. coli: review and new insights from an “inverse” deuterium isotope effect. Biochim Biophys Acta 1655: 64–70.
  51. 51. Kennis JT, Crosson S, Gauden M, van Stokkum IH, Moffat K, et al. (2003) Primary reactions of the LOV2 domain of phototropin, a plant blue-light photoreceptor. Biochemistry 42: 3385–3392.
  52. 52. Rutherford AW, Thurnauer MC (1982) Radical pair state in photosystem II. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 79: 7283–7287.
  53. 53. Sakmar TP, Menon ST, Marin EP, Awad ES (2002) Rhodopsin: insights from recent structural studies. Annu Rev Biophys Biomol Struct 31: 443–484.
  54. 54. Gegear RJ, Casselman A, Waddell S, Reppert SM (2008) Cryptochrome mediates light-dependent magnetosensitivity in Drosophila. Nature 454: 1014–1018.
  55. 55. Wehner R, Labhart T (1970) Perception of the geomagnetic field in the fly Drosophila melanogaster. Experientia 26: 967–968.
  56. 56. Wiltschko R, Wiltschko W (2006) Magnetoreception. Bioessays 28: 157–168.
  57. 57. Zhu H, Sauman I, Yuan Q, Casselman A, Emery-Le M, et al. (2008) Cryptochromes define a novel circadian clock mechanism in monarch butterflies that may underlie sun compass navigation. PLoS Biol 6: e4.
  58. 58. Blau J, Young MW (1999) Cycling vrille expression is required for a functional Drosophila clock. Cell 99: 661–671.
  59. 59. Helfrich-Förster C (1998) Robust circadian rhythmicity of Drosophila melanogaster requires the presence of lateral neurons: a brain-behavioral study of disconnected mutants. J Comp Physiol A 182: 435–453.
  60. 60. Rieger D, Fraunholz C, Popp J, Bichler D, Dittmann R, et al. (2007) The fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster favors dim light and times its activity peaks to early dawn and late dusk. J Biol Rhythms 22: 387–399.
  61. 61. Sokolove PG, Bushell WN (1978) The chi-square periodogram: its utility for analysis of circadian rhythms. J Theor Biol 72: 131–160.