Skip to main content
Advertisement
  • Loading metrics

Candidate Gustatory Interneurons Modulating Feeding Behavior in the Drosophila Brain

Abstract

Feeding is a fundamental activity of all animals that can be regulated by internal energy status or external sensory signals. We have characterized a zinc finger transcription factor, klumpfuss (klu), which is required for food intake in Drosophila larvae. Microarray analysis indicates that expression of the neuropeptide gene hugin (hug) in the brain is altered in klu mutants and that hug itself is regulated by food signals. Neuroanatomical analysis demonstrates that hug-expressing neurons project axons to the pharyngeal muscles, to the central neuroendocrine organ, and to the higher brain centers, whereas hug dendrites are innervated by external gustatory receptor-expressing neurons, as well as by internal pharyngeal chemosensory organs. The use of tetanus toxin to block synaptic transmission of hug neurons results in alteration of food intake initiation, which is dependent on previous nutrient condition. Our results provide evidence that hug neurons function within a neural circuit that modulates taste-mediated feeding behavior.

Introduction

All animals must be able to evaluate their nutrient requirement, as well as the nutrient supply offered by the environment, and translate the resulting information into appropriate behavioral responses. These can range from deciding to stop or continue feeding, or to look for alternate food sources. The nutrient signals can derive internally, reflecting the body's energy state and metabolic need, or through external sensory inputs, such as olfactory and gustatory signals. The sensory modalities further provide the basis for many types of higher brain functions, such as learning and memory. Feeding behavior, in turn, decisively influences almost all aspects of animal growth and reproduction. The role of the central nervous system (CNS) in integrating an animal's feeding behavior with sensory signals on the availability and quality of nutrients is, although undisputed, insufficiently understood [1].

Drosophila provides a genetically accessible system to study the molecular mechanisms that coordinate feeding behavior with sensory signals. This organism has an array of feeding characteristics that can be exploited for behavioral analysis, and insects in general have been used extensively as models for a wide range of behavioral and physiological studies [2,3]. In this context, the identification of genes encoding chemosensory receptors in Drosophila has provided a major impetus in understanding sensory signal transduction [48]. These genes have been broadly divided as encoding olfactory or gustatory receptors (ORs and GRs, respectively). Olfactory sensory neurons expressing specific ORs in the external mouth region project axons to distinct glomeruli of the antennal lobe [812]. Projection neurons then connect the antennal lobe to the mushroom body, where central processing of olfactory information occurs [1315]. Gustatory sensory neurons are located not only in the external mouth region, but also internally in the pharynx; both types project to the subesophageal ganglion (SOG), a region implicated in feeding and taste response [8,1618]. As compared with the antennal lobe, much less is known about the organization of the SOG—for example, whether it is also organized in glomerular structure. The neurons that connect the SOG to higher brain centers, in a manner analogous to the olfactory projection neurons, have also not been identified.

In both olfactory and gustatory cases, the knowledge is even sparser concerning the identity of interneurons that act between the sensory neurons and motor or neuroendocrine outputs and how they might influence feeding behavior. Studies in different insects have shown that various parts of the CNS are interconnected with the neuroendocrine organs and the enteric (stomatogastric) nervous system, which innervates the feeding apparatus [19,20]. The mouth parts have also been shown to be innervated by nerves from the SOG [21]. Nevertheless, an integrated map of the neurons comprising these circuits and their function in mediating a behavioral response has been lacking.

We have previously identified a gene, pumpless (ppl), that is required for food intake behavior in the Drosophila larvae [22]. It encodes a subunit of the glycine cleavage system and is expressed exclusively in the fat body. Although not feeding, ppl mutant larvae do not show characteristics of starving larvae, as assayed both by molecular markers and behavioral characteristics; furthermore, feeding high levels of amino acids can phenocopy several aspects of the ppl feeding phenotype. These observations led to a model in which amino acid-dependent signals from the fat body to the brain can signal cessation of feeding. In this study, we characterize another mutant, klumpfuss (klu), with a phenotype very similar to ppl mutants. Through microarray analysis, we identified the neuropeptide gene hugin (hug) as being deregulated in klu mutants. hug is expressed in a small assembly of about 20 neurons in the SOG. Connectivity mapping and behavioral studies suggest that hug-expressing neurons function in a neural circuitry in the brain that modulates chemosensory signal-dependent feeding behavior.

Results

Molecular Characterization of Larval Mutant Defective in Feeding Behavior

In a screen for Drosophila mutant larvae defective in feeding, we identified the P-element line P(9036). These animals fail to pump food from the pharynx into the esophagus (Figure 1A), which is not due to a morphological block in the esophagus. The failure to feed is also not due to a general illness of the animal or global locomotory defects, because they can move around with the same vigor as wild-type or heterozygote siblings. P(9036) larvae also display wandering-like behavior, in which they move away from the food (Figure 1B and 1C). During this wandering-like phase, P(9036) larvae move about with food lodged in their pharynx, further supporting the view that the feeding defect is not due to a general body movement defect. Wandering behavior is observed in wild-type larvae when they stop feeding and move away from food shortly before pupariation [23]. These feeding behavior defects have also been observed for ppl mutants [22]. ppl encodes an amino acid catabolizing enzyme that is expressed exclusively in the fat body, an organ analogous to the vertebrate liver. Thus, P(9036) and ppl mutants, as immature first instar larvae, display feeding behaviors characteristic of sated, full-grown, third instar larvae. We characterized the gene corresponding to P(9036) and found it to be klu, a zinc finger protein-encoding gene that is expressed specifically in the developing nervous system [24,25]. P(9036) fails to complement the lethality of all klu alleles tested, and trans-heterozygotes also show the characteristic feeding defect (Figure 1D).

thumbnail
Figure 1. Phenotypic Characterization and Expression Analysis of P(9036) Mutants

(A) P(9036)-mutant larvae show feeding and growth defects as compared to wild-type (wt) controls. In wild-type, red food fills the gut, whereas it accumulates in the pharynx of P(9036) mutants. Physically, the food can pass the mutant pharynx (see arrow).

(B) P(9036) mutants show a wandering behavior, in which they leave the food source (depicted in a schematic snapshot drawing; dots represent larvae outside the food source, which is shown in red).

(C) Quantification of the P(9036) wandering phenotype shows that from 50 individuals, 23 ± 4 mutants can be found outside the food source, in contrast to only 2 ± 1 for the wild-type control at a given time point (n = 8 time points).

(D) The known klu alleles kluXR19, kluR51C, and P(1741) fail to complement P(9036), and trans-heterozygous larvae display the feeding phenotype.

(E) Microarray analysis of P(9036) revealed several neuropeptide genes as being deregulated in P(9036) mutants compared to wild-type controls (×-fold changes in P(9036) are listed; genes with more than 2-fold change are boxed).

(F–H) Semi-quantitative in situ hybridizations to first instar larval brains verify the upregulation of the neuropeptide gene hug in P(9036) klu mutants in comparison to wild-type controls, and show an upregulation of hug in ppl feeding mutants as well. The expression of hug is in the SOG. Anterior points to the upper left corner; ventral views are shown.

(I–K) Semi-quantitative in situ hybridizations to late second instar larval brains demonstrate a downregulation of hug in wild-type under starvation (PBS) and sugar conditions (20% sucrose), in comparison to normal feeding condition (yeast). Anterior points to the upper left corner; ventral views are shown.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0030305.g001

The Neuropeptide Gene hug Expression is Altered in klu Mutants and in Amino Acid-Deficient Conditions

To study the central control process that could underlie the feeding defect of klu mutants, we performed microarray analysis of klu mutants with a focus on neuropeptide genes. We reasoned that their expression patterns in the brain would be specific enough for analysis at single-cell resolution. Furthermore, neuropeptides have been shown to influence food intake in different organisms, including mammals [1]. RNA from klu mutant larvae and wild-type larvae were isolated and hybridized to three Affymetrix chips each and compared. Figure 1E lists Drosophila neuropeptide genes and their expression profile in klu mutants, relative to wild-type first instar larvae. We then performed in situ hybridizations on wild-type larval brains with the six highest upregulated genes. We decided to focus our efforts on hug because it had the most specific expression pattern in the larval brain. While all others showed staining in different parts of the brain or in the ventral nerve cord (VNC) (unpublished data), hug showed staining in only a cluster of about 20 cells in the SOG of the larval brain, with no staining anywhere else (Figures 1F and 2). Its expression in embryos is also highly restricted in the brain [26]. hug encodes a prepropeptide capable of generating at least two neuropeptides, Drm-PK2 and hug-γ. The former encodes a myostimulatory peptide while the latter shows homology to ecdysis-triggering hormone-1, ETH-1 [26]. Both can activate a G-protein–coupled receptor belonging to the vertebrate neuromedin U group [27]. A hug homolog is also found in Anopheles gambiae [28].

thumbnail
Figure 2. Neuroanatomical Analysis of hug Expression and Neuronal Projection Patterns in Larvae

(A) A schematic drawing of the Drosophila larval CNS, showing the relative positions of the two brain hemispheres (BH) and the VNC, with the neuroendocrine ring gland (RG) located dorsoanterior to the CNS. The esophagus goes through the brain, and we have termed the hole through which the esophagus passes as the foramen (F). Positions of the SOG, the larval antennal lobes (AL), the mushroom bodies (MB), and the median neurosecretory cells (mNSC) are depicted.

(B) hug expression (shown in green) is restricted to the SOG. adipokinetic hormone (akh, shown in red) serves as a ring gland marker; the CNS is false-colored in blue.

(C) Detection of marker gene expression under hug promoter-Gal4 (hugS3) construct (shown in green) labels hug cell bodies and neuronal projections relative to the neuropil (22C10, shown in red) and nuclei of post-mitotic neurons (elav, shown in blue).

(D–F) The endogenous hug expression pattern (shown in red in [D]) is reproduced by the hugS3 expression pattern (shown in [E]). Note double-positive cell bodies in (F). akh serves as ring gland marker; CNS, ring gland, and foramen are outlined.

(G and H) Immunofluorescent (G) as well as direct (H) detection of GFP expressed under the hug promoter-Ga14 construct reveals putative hug dendrites dorsoanterior of the hug cell bodies, where spherical structures are innervated. The hug cell bodies in the depicted blow-up of SOG region are out of focus in (H).

(I) hug axons innervating the protocerebrum cross the midline, showing ipsilateral and contralateral innervation of mushroom body region.

(J–L) Expression of eGFP under hugS3 shows hug axons leaving the SOG laterally (see arrows). These can be followed to the cephalopharyngeal complex of the larvae (K and L) onto the pharyngeal muscles (axons shown in green overlayed with transmission light picture; note the mouth hook apparatus in black). Dorsal (K) and lateral views (L) are shown, anterior points to upper left corner.

All fluorescence images in this and subsequent figures are Z-stack projections, and all scales bars are 50 μm, unless otherwise noted.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0030305.g002

To confirm the microarray data, we performed semi-quantitative in situ hybridization in wild-type and klu mutant larval brains. hug is upregulated in klu mutants (Figure 1F and 1G). We then investigated whether hug expression is also regulated in ppl larvae, which display a similar feeding defect as klu. There is also an upregulation of hug in ppl mutants (Figure 1H). We next investigated whether hug expression is regulated by different nutrient signals. We therefore placed wild-type larvae in starvation and sugar conditions (that is, both being amino acid-deficient diets) and monitored hug expression. hug was downregulated in both conditions (Figure 1I, 1J, and 1K), indicating a response to nutrient signals distinct from simple lack of energy. As hug is upregulated in klu and in ppl mutants, both of which do not feed and wander about, a higher hug level correlates with decrease of food intake and food-seeking behavior. For hug downregulation under starvation and sugar conditions, a lower hug level correlates with increased food-seeking behavior since Drosophila larvae become hyperactive and disperse when food is removed.

hug-Expressing Neurons Project to the Ring Gland, the Pharyngeal Muscles, and the Protocerebrum

As mentioned above, hug is expressed specifically in a small group of neurons in the SOG (Figure 2A and 2B). To gain insight into the physiological processes that hug-expressing neurons (referred to as hug neurons) could be involved in, we wanted to determine their connectivity pattern. Therefore, we constructed a hug promoter-Gal4 line (hugS3) in order to express different versions of green fluorescent protein (GFP) marker genes for neuroanatomical studies. This approach revealed hug neuron projection to the ring gland (Figure 2C). The ring gland, as the master neuroendocrine organ of Drosophila larvae, controls metabolism and growth. For example, median neurosecretory cells of the pars intercerebralis that express Drosophila insulin like peptides (dilps), also project to the ring gland, whereas adipokinetic hormone (akh), thought to be a glucagon homolog, is produced by the ring gland [2931].

In addition to the ring gland, we also observed hug neuron projection to the protocerebrum, near the median neurosecretory cells and the mushroom bodies (Figure 2D–2F), which comprise the center for olfactory learning and memory [13,32]. The axons projecting to the protocerebrum also cross at the midline just above the foramen (Figure 2I). We also noticed an intriguing glomerular-like structure of what are most likely hug dendrites in the SOG, just dorsoanterior to the hug cell bodies (Figure 2G and 2H). Singh [17] has described glomerular organization in the SOG of adult Drosophila that relays gustatory information. Such a glomerular organization has not been previously recognized for the larval SOG, but it would be analogous to the glomerular organization in the antennal lobes that relay olfactory information.

Strikingly, there is also projection of hug neurons to the pharyngeal muscles (Figure 2J, 2K, and 2L), which pump food into the mouth atrium. These arise from axons that leave the brain (Figure 2J) and project anteriorly along each side of the dorsal pharyngeal muscles, and terminate near the anterior end of the pharynx (Figures 2K, 2L, and 4). There has been no previous case of identified neurons in the larval SOG that project to motor outputs. At this point, we do not know whether the pharyngeal muscles are actually innervated by these axons. Taken together, these results demonstrate that hug neurons in the larvae project to key organs regulating feeding and growth—namely, the pharynx and the ring gland—as well as to higher brain centers.

hug Dendrites Innervate GR-Expressing Sensory Organs and Chemosensory Organs of the Pharynx

The projection of hug neurons to the mushroom body region, together with the fact that hug is expressed specifically in the SOG, which relays gustatory information, suggested that hug neurons could be involved in mediating chemosensory signals. Therefore, we investigated whether hug neurons receive direct input from the chemosensory organs (Figure 3A–3D). It has been demonstrated that sensory organs in the larval head that express ORs or GRs send their axons either to the antennal lobe or the SOG [8] (Figure 3A and 3B). Recently, an enhancer trap line MJ94 was used to label putative chemosensory organs of the internal pharynx [18]. As internal pharyngeal sensory organs are good candidates for transducing gustatory signals, we wondered if these sensory organs terminate at hug dendrites. As shown in Figure 3E and 3F, they indeed terminate in the contact region of hug dendrites.

thumbnail
Figure 3. hug Neurons Receive Gustatory Input

(A) Schematic drawing of the head region of a Drosophila larva with external as well as internal chemosensory neurons in antennomaxillary complex and internal mouth region innervating the larval CNS (depicted in green). hug neurons in the SOG (shown in red) project to the ring gland (RG), pharyngeal muscle (PM) region, and the protocerebrum (PC). Relative positions of external chemosensory sensillae (dorsal organ [do] and terminal organ [to]), internal chemosensory sensillae (ventral pharyngeal sense organ [vps], dorsal pharyngeal sense organ [dps], dorsal pharyngeal organ [dpo], and posterior pharyngeal sense organ [pps]), and major projections to the CNS are shown.

(B) GR21D1-positive sensory neurons (shown by X-Gal staining) in the dorsal organ project axons to the CNS (see arrow).

(C and D) Optical section through median CNS (composed of ten confocal 1-μm sections) shows hug arborizations in the SOG and mushroom body region (shown in green) relative to general neuropil (red) and cortical (DNA marker Draq5, blue) landmarks. Note the labeling of corpora allata cells in the ring gland (arrow; see Materials and Methods and Figure S2). Boxed area is shown at higher magnification in (D), revealing spherically organized neuropil regions lateral to foramen (partially outlined).

(E) Expression of nSyb-GFP under MJ94 enhancer trap construct labels axon terminals of internal gustatory sensory neurons (shown in green), innervating SOG and VNC.

(F) Close-up of SOG region shows co-localization of spherically organized axon terminals of MJ94 positive gustatory neurons (green) and hug neuronal arborizations (red).

(G) Axon terminals of GR66C1-positive chemosensory neurons (shown in green) can be detected in the vicinity of hug cell bodies (shown in red). dilp3 staining (blue) serves as morphological landmark.

(H) Optical section (composed of ten confocal 1-μm sections) containing the GR66C1 axon terminals (shown in green) also include the spherical hug arborizations. hug cell bodies are out of focus in (H); a close-up of the SOG is depicted.

(I and J) Axon terminals of GR21D1 positive chemosensory neurons (shown in green) also project to the vicinity of hug cell bodies (shown in red in [I]), but the optical section comprising these terminals does not contain the hug arborizations (note the absence of spherical hug dendrites in [J] as compared to [H]).

(K–M) Comparison of axon tracts used by olfactory projection neurons, which can be labeled by enhancer trap line GH146 (shown by GFP real color in [K] and in green in [L], marked by asterisks), with those used by hug neurons (shown by YFP real color in [K] and in red in [L], marked by arrows), indicate that hug neurons are distinct from olfactory projection neurons (OPN). These differences are summarized in (M). LAN, larval antennal nerve; LMN, larval maxillary nerve; LPN, larval pharyngeal nerve.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0030305.g003

To further test this, we checked to see if chemosensory neurons that express specific GRs also project to hug dendrites. For example, it has been shown that GR66C1-positive neurons project to the SOG, whereas GR21D1-positive neurons project to the antennal lobe [8]. To see if these sensory projections terminate at or near hug neurons, we first performed staining of GR66C1- and GR21D1-positive axon terminals with hug in situ hybridization. GR66C1 receptor neurons indeed project to the vicinity of hug-expressing cells (Figure 3G). To see if these axons may potentially make synaptic contacts with hug dendrites, we used a hug promoter–yellow fluorescent protein (YFP) line (in which YFP was placed directly under the hug promoter), in combination with GR promoters driving nSyb-GFP [8,33], thus allowing simultaneous visualization of GR axon terminals and hug dendrites. As shown in Figure 3H, GR66C1-positive neurons project to the glomerular-like SOG region contacted by hug dendrites. GR21D1-positive neurons also project near the hug cells (Figure 3I), but by contrast to GR66C1-positive neurons, do not contact hug dendrites (Figure 3J). Rather, they terminate dorsoanterior to the hug dendrites, where the antennal lobes are located [8]. Taken together, these results suggested that hug neurons may act as second-order interneurons that relay gustatory information.

To further distinguish the relationship between hug neurons and the olfactory or gustatory systems, we determined whether hug neurons share the same axon tracts to the mushroom bodies as the second-order neurons that relay olfactory sensory input. The dendrites of these olfactory projection neurons underlie the glomerular structure of the antennal lobes and vertically transduce olfactory information for processing to the mushroom bodies. These projections can be visualized by the enhancer trap line GH146 [14]. As shown in Figure 3K and 3L, the axon projections of hug neurons are distinct from olfactory projection neurons: hug neurons project to a more dorsomedial region in the protocerebrum than olfactory projection neurons, and they use different axon tracts. These results essentially rule out hug neurons being olfactory projection neurons. Projection neurons transducing gustatory signals to higher brain centers have not yet been identified. In this context, hug neurons could act as gustatory projection neurons that connect gustatory sensory neurons via SOG with the protocerebrum (Figure 3M).

Subpopulation of hug Neurons Project to Distinct Targets

We have also noticed a difference in the projection specificity among the hug neurons. A series of enhancer trap lines have been isolated that label cells projecting their axons to the ring gland [34], one of which (Okt30) is co-expressed with hug (Figure 4A and 4B). When we use our hug promoter-YFP line (to distinguish it from GFP reporter constructs) in combination with the Okt30 ring gland enhancer trap line, we find that a distinct set of hug neurons project to only the ring gland and not to the protocerebrum, the pharynx, or the ventral cord (Figure 4C).

thumbnail
Figure 4. Subpopulations of hug Neurons Innervate Distinct Targets

(A and B) Enhancer trap line Okt30 (shown in green) labels SOG neurons projecting to ring gland. Okt30 expression pattern colocalizes with hug expression (shown in red in [B]). There are four double positive cells in (B). dilp3 staining (blue) serves as morphological landmark.

(C) Direct detection and false colorization of GFP expressed in Okt30 positive cells (green) and YFP expressed under hug promoter (red) reveals only the axons to ring gland as double positive.

(D) TH promoter construct labels dopaminergic CNS neurons (shown in green). Four TH-positive SOG neurons also express hug (shown in red). capa staining (blue) serves as morphological landmark.

(E) Combination of projection patterns of TH positive cells (green) with hug cells (red) reveals the axons innervating the pharyngeal muscles as the only double positive ones.

(F) Schematic summary of (A–E) showing distinct subpopulations of hug neurons projecting to the ring gland only (green), pharynx only (blue), and the remaining targets (red).

(G–I) Projection pattern of hug is unaffected in klu mutants. Direct detection and false coloring of hug promoter-YFP (shown in green) in the klu background reveals the pharyngeal muscles (PM), the ring gland (RG), and the VNC as being targeted in feeding mutants. Trachea are false-colored (red) in composite figure (I).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0030305.g004

Another subpopulation of hug neurons is revealed by using the TH-Gal4 line. This drives reporter gene expression under the promoter of tyrosine hydroxylase (TH), a key enzyme in dopamine synthesis [35]. As shown in Figure 4D, specific hug neurons express TH-Gal4 reporter gene, indicating that a subset of hug neurons might be dopaminergic. When TH-Gal4-driven lacZ is used in combination with hug promoter-YFP, we observe that TH-positive hug neurons project to only the pharynx and not to the protocerebrum, the ring gland, or the VNC (Figure 4E). These results indicate that at least three distinct subpopulations of hug neurons exist: those projecting to only the ring gland, those projecting to only the pharyngeal muscles, and those projecting to the protocerebrum and/or the VNC (Figure 4F). The distinct target specificity suggests differences in the function of the hug subpopulations. In the honeybee Apis, the subesophageal-calycal tract neurons are located in the SOG, send axons to the protocerebrum, and receive input from the sensory neurons of the proboscis; these neurons are thought to transduce gustatory information [36]. Some of the hug neurons could act similarly to these honeybee neurons. Based on the connectivity map of the hug neurons, we also wondered if the global targeting of these neurons was altered in klu mutant larvae. Therefore, we crossed the hug promoter-YFP construct into klu mutant background (Figures 4G and 4H). Although we cannot rule out subtle local differences, the basic connectivity pattern is retained in the mutants (Figure 4I).

hug Neuron Connectivity Pattern Is Similar in Larvae and Adults

To see if hug neurons might also have a function in the adults, we determined the connectivity pattern in adult animals. There are some noticeable morphological differences in the feeding apparatus and neuroendocrine organs between adults and larvae (Figure 5A). One is the presence of the crop in the adult but not in the larva. The crop is a food storage organ, and its absence in the larvae most likely reflects a difference in the feeding habits; whereas adults are intermittent feeders, the larvae feed continuously. Another is the relocation of the neuroendocrine organs. The corpora cardiaca/corpora allata (CC/CA) complex, which comprises part of the ring gland in the larvae, is located right above the proventriculus in the adults, at the junction between the gut and the crop. This is in contrast to the larvae, where it is located on top of the brain hemispheres. Monitoring hugS3 expression in adults, we observe axon projections to the protocerebrum, the CC/CA complex, and the ventral cord (Figure 5B–5H). A subpopulation of hug neurons may also be dopaminergic, as in the larvae (Figure 5I). To further characterize the projections to the protocerebrum, we used the OK107 enhancer trap Gal4 line [37] together with hug promoter-YFP. These stainings indicate that hug axons traverse along the median neurosecretory cells in the pars intercerebralis, and terminate near the mushroom bodies (Figure 5J and 5K). A similar pattern is observed in the larvae (Figure 5L and 5M). The precise targets of hug neurons projecting to the protocerebrum remain to be determined. Taken together, despite the morphological differences, the connectivity pattern of hug neurons is remarkably similar between larvae and adults.

thumbnail
Figure 5. Neuroanatomical Analysis of hug Interneurons in Adults

(A) Relative positions of adult CNS composed of brain hemispheres (BH) and VNC, as well as esophagus passing the brain through the foramen (F), proventriculus (P), crop (C), and gut (G) are depicted schematically. The neuroendocrine CC/CA complex is located on top of the proventriculus (arrow).

(B) Localization of hug neurons and projections in adult CNS relative to nuclear marker. Note projections to the protocerebrum (top of the head), VNC, and CC/CA (arrow).

(C) hug neuronal projections relative to general neuropil marker (22c10, shown in red).

(D–F) Close-ups of different optical sections (composed of confocal 1- to 2.5-μm sections) show spherical hug arborizations in median SOG region (green in [D]), arborizations in lateral SOG region (green in [E], arrow) and in protocerebrum (green in [F]).

(G) Transmission light image of CC/CA complex (arrow) on top of proventriculus (P).

(H) Immunofluorescent close-up of the area boxed in (G). Nuclei of elav positive CC cells (blue) and of CA (green, see arrow), hug axon terminals (green, see arrowhead).

(I) TH positive SOG neurons (green) co-express hug (red); capa staining serves as morphological landmark.

(J and K) Different optical sections (composed of confocal 1- to 2.5-mm sections) show hug (green) projections above the mushroom bodies and adjacent to the median neurosecretory cells marked by OK107 (mushroom body and median neurosecretory cell marker, red); elav marker, blue. (K) Close-up of the region in (J), showing the medial neurosecretory cells. Scale bars equal 20 μ.

(L and M) Different optical sections (composed of confocal 1-to 2.5-mm sections) showing OK107 (green) and hug (red) in larva. (M) Close-up of the median neurosecretory cell region. Scale bars equal 20 μ.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0030305.g005

Blocking Synaptic Transmission of hug Neurons Alters Food Intake Behavior

Based on the connectivity map of hug neurons and the alteration in hug expression under different nutrient and feeding conditions, we initiated a series of experiments to explore the role of hug in regulating feeding. As hug mutants have not yet been identified, we tested the effects of overexpressing hug in the larvae. We first used hugS3 to drive hug expression but did not observe any phenotype (unpublished data). This is most likely because using an endogenous promoter does not result in high enough overexpression of hug in cells that already express physiological levels of hug. We therefore used a strong ubiquitous promoter (tubulin-gal4). There was a strong reduction in growth (Figure 6A), with no larvae surviving to pupal stage; we also observed defects in food intake, although not to the same strong degree as with klu mutants (Figure 6A). This is consistent with the view outlined earlier that high hug levels correlate with decreased food intake.

thumbnail
Figure 6. Overexpression of hug and Blocking hug Synaptic Transmission Causes Feeding Phenotypes

(A) Overexpression of hug neuropeptide gene under ubiquitous promoter leads to reduced feeding and growth (compare size of UAS-hug larvae and controls of same age) as well as to larval lethality. Note the individual phenocopying the klu feeding phenotype (arrow).

(B) Partial rescue of klu feeding defect by blocking hug neuronal activity. n= 5; error bars represent standard deviation; see Materials and Methods for details.

(C) Schematic of adult internal morphology with proventriculus, crop, and gut. Crop is depicted in full and empty state.

(D) Feeding behavior of adult flies monitored by the presence of red food in the midgut and crop (marked by arrows). Starting from the same feeding status (empty crop at time point 0 min), experimental flies expressing TeTxLC under hugS3 construct initiate uptake of red food immediately (notice large amount of red food in crop and gut after 5 min) when confronted with red yeast paste after feeding on standard fly food overnight (overnight normal). In contrast, control flies (hugS3 flies and TeTxLC flies crossed with wild-type) initiate food uptake after 15–35 min (notice traces of red food in midgut) when confronted with red yeast paste. The same feeding status is detected in experimental as well as in control flies after long feeding period on red yeast (overnight normal, 180 min). Overnight starvation equalizes feeding behavior when flies are confronted with red yeast paste for 15 min (notice same amount of red food in all cases at overnight starvation 15 min). Two representative samples of each time point and genotype are displayed. Two independent experiments were carried out, and for each experimental set ten individuals were taken out randomly and dissected. From 20 total flies, 16 ± 2 showed the phenotype displayed here o/n, overnight.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0030305.g006

In order to gain further information on the function of hug neurons, we then blocked synaptic transmission in these cells using tetanus toxin light chain (TeTxLC) [38]. We first carried out the experiments in the larvae but did not see any difference (unpublished data). However, we reasoned that any potential increase in feeding response may not be readily detectable in the larvae because they feed continuously, already at a maximal rate. Therefore, we tested whether blocking synaptic transmission of hug neurons could suppress the feeding defect of klu mutants. We indeed observed a significant rescue of klu mutant feeding phenotype (Figure 6B).

We then carried out behavioral analysis on adults, since they are discontinuous feeders and thus may display an increased feeding behavior. Furthermore, one can visualize the quantity of food eaten by the size of the crop (Figure 6C). Experimental and control flies were placed in food vials containing standard fly food for several days. They were then transferred to yeast paste containing red dye. A striking result was observed. After 5 min, the experimental flies had a completely filled crop (Figure 6D, left column), whereas the control lines (Figure 6D, middle and right columns) had very little food in the crop. Even after 30 min, the control flies had very little food in their crops and only traces of red food were detectable in the midgut. By 180 min, both experimental and control flies showed the same degree of feeding. These results suggested that hug neurons are involved in regulating the initiation phase of feeding: control flies wait for a certain period before initiating feeding on the new food source, whereas decreasing hug neuronal signaling results in flies initiating their feeding immediately. When flies were transferred from yeast to colored yeast, or normal food to colored normal food, no difference was seen between experimental and control flies (unpublished data), indicating that hug neurons are not simply affecting the rate of feeding per se; we also did not observe a difference when transferring from yeast to normal food, indicating that the hug neuron-dependent behavioral effect is also not due to a simple fact of changing food sources. When flies were transferred from normal food into yeast containing 1M quinine (quinine is an aversive tastant), experimental flies again filled their crops earlier than controls (Figure S1). However, when flies were kept on yeast containing 1M quinine, and then transferred to yeast without the quinine, both experimental and control flies filled their crops with the new yeast within 5 min (Figure S1); analogously, when flies were starved before placing them on red yeast, both control and experimental flies filled their crops at about the same rate (Figure 6D, bottom row). These results suggest that the quality of previous food condition plays a role in defining hug neuronal function. Taken together, our studies support the view that hug neurons act within a neural circuitry in the brain that modulates feeding behavior based on chemosensory and nutrient signals.

Discussion

Central Relay of Gustatory Information

The identification of candidate chemosensory receptors in mammals and invertebrates has provided major insights into the molecular mechanisms underlying sensory information processing. In the Drosophila olfactory system, projections of OR-expressing sensory organs terminate at specific glomerular structures in the antennal lobe. The olfactory projection neurons then act in a second relay to convey the information to the mushroom bodies in the higher brain region. The gustatory organs, expressing specific GRs, project to a different brain region, the SOG, which has been implicated in gustatory signal transduction and feeding response in different insects. Our results indicate that the neurons that express the hug neuropeptide gene are likely candidates for acting as interneurons that transduce gustatory information. These comprise an assembly of about 20 neurons in the SOG. The close proximity of their dendrites with the axon terminals of gustatory sensory organs of the external head, and chemosensory organs of the internal pharynx, suggests a synaptic contact, but this requires functional verification. Whether the SOG is also organized into glomerular structure, like the antennal lobe, is not known. Such an organization has been suggested in adult Drosophila [17], although data on larvae have been lacking. Our results on the dendritic pattern of hug neurons also suggest a glomerular structure of the larval SOG, but this remains an open issue.

The hug neurons, in turn, send axons to at least three distinct targets: the ring gland, the pharyngeal muscles, and the protocerebrum. The projections to the ring gland and the pharyngeal muscles suggest that hug neurons coordinate sensory information with growth, metabolism, and food intake; the axon tracts to the protocerebrum suggest a role of hug neurons in transducing sensory signals for processing in the higher brain centers. These axon tracts are distinct from those of the olfactory projection neurons, projecting to a more dorsomedial region of the mushroom body, and adjacent to the median neurosecretory cells of the pars intercerebralis. Thus, hug neurons are ideally connected to undertake the role of integrating gustatory sensory signals with higher brain functions and feeding behavior.

Chemosensory Adaptation, Nutrient Status, and Food Intake Response

The chemosensory systems of all animals play critical roles in modulating feeding behavioral response. Feeding behavior can have diverse aspects, including locating a food source, evaluating food for nutritional appropriateness, choosing between different food sources, and deciding to initiate or terminate feeding. Blocking synaptic transmission by tetanus toxin in the hug neurons alters a specific aspect of the feeding behavioral response. When transferred to a certain new food source, the control flies wait for a period before initiating feeding, whereas experimental flies start feeding almost immediately. In both cases, the size of the crop after a longer feeding period does not change, meaning that no difference is seen in the termination phase of feeding. It is interesting to note that GR66C1 (also named GR66a) neurons, which project to hug dendrites, have recently been shown to mediate aversive taste response [39,40]. This is consistent with the behavior of flies in which hug signaling is decreased, since they lose their “aversive” response, as manifested in the elimination of a wait period before feeding. This behavior is dependent on internal nutrient status, as well as food quality, since if animals are starved or given food with an aversive tastant beforehand (such as yeast with quinine), control flies also start feeding immediately on the new yeast source.

Insects have evolved a wide variety of feeding behaviors based on food identity, quality, and availability. Some of these are innate, whereas others are acquired through experience. For example, food preference in the tobacco hornworm is dependent on what they initially encounter after hatching. They are capable of growing on a wide variety of sources, but once they have fed on a particular food type, they will maintain this food preference [41,42]. In this context, a possible scenario is that Drosophila associate feeding with a particular food source with which they become familiar. When they encounter a different food source, they must first re-evaluate it, perhaps for nutrient content, or adapt to it, before initiating feeding. Therefore, hug neurons appear to regulate the decision to initiate feeding based on previous food experience.

Central Integration of Feeding Behavior and Growth

In animals with a developed endocrine system, there is an intricate interdependence among feeding, growth, and neuroendocrine activity. Drosophila larvae are characterized by continuous feeding and a huge increase in organismal growth; in the adult, although no growth at organismal level takes place, a large cellular growth is required in the female for egg production. Both are highly dependent on feeding and the quality of food, such as protein content, and are under neuroendocrine control [2]. klu and ppl represent two genes that are required for food intake and growth in Drosophila. Mutations in both genes result in reduced food intake and growth. In addition, as young larvae, mutants display a wandering-like behavior, which is reminiscent of full-grown wild-type larvae, which stop feeding and move away from the food source just prior to pupariation, a process dependent on the neuroendocrine system [23]. Mutations in either of the genes lead to an upregulation of hug neuropeptide gene expression in the brain, whereas hug expression is downregulated in the absence of food signals.

What could be the function of the hug neuropeptides? hug encodes at least two distinct neuropeptides [26]. One (hug-γ) has homology to an ecdysone triggering hormone, while the second (Drm-PK-2) is a pyrokinin with myostimulatory activity. hug-γ could be involved in controlling growth and metabolism. This view is supported by projection of hug neurons to the ring gland, the master neuroendocrine organ. In addition, overexpression of hug has been shown to cause molting defects [26]. Drm-PK-2, on the other hand, may play a role in modifying the mechanical aspect of food intake, which is supported by the projection of hug neurons to the pharyngeal muscles. One interesting possibility is that the different neuropeptides are translated or trafficked to different targets in subset of hug neurons. In this case, a common gene expression pattern can be utilized to send out different signals to the different targets, such as to the higher brain center, feeding apparatus, and neuroendocrine organ. This would be a mechanism for coordinating different growth-dependent processes with a common input signal, for example, from a particular food signal. In this context, one way to explain the upregulation of the hug gene in klu and ppl mutants would be that the level of hug gene differentially correlates with the degree of food-seeking response. High levels, as in the mutants that do not feed, would reflect lower feeding and food-seeking response, whereas low levels, as in the absence of food sensory input, would reflect increased food-seeking response. This would also be consistent with hug overexpression studies and with the correlation seen between decreasing hug neuronal activity and increased feeding (Figure 7). Further functional studies, including imaging analysis [43,44], should increase our understanding of how the hug neural circuit coordinates sensory perception, feeding behavior, and growth.

thumbnail
Figure 7. Model of hug as Modulator of Feeding Behavior

The hug neurons, which express the neuropeptide gene hug and which interconnect gustatory sensillae via the SOG to the pharyngeal muscles, the protocerebrum, and the neuroendocrine organ, modulate chemosensory dependent feeding behavior. Increased hug signaling correlates with decreased feeding, whereas decreased hug signaling correlates with increased feeding (see Discussion section for details).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0030305.g007

Materials and Methods

Feeding behavior assay

The larval feeding behavior assay was done as described previously [22]. Flies were allowed to lay eggs on apple juice agar plates containing colored yeast paste (150 mg Carmen Red, Sigma-Aldrich [St. Louis, Missouri, United States] per 100 g yeast paste). Given numbers of larvae from overnight egg collections were allowed to develop for 24 h at 25 °C and subsequently (2-h intervals) monitored for feeding and wandering phenotypes under a dissection microscope. For starvation experiments, wild-type larvae of late second instar were placed in petri dishes, containing filter paper that was soaked with either PBS (for complete starvation) or PBS containing 20% sucrose. For normal feeding conditions, fresh yeast paste was given. All feeding experiments were done at room temperature for 6 h. Overexpression studies were done using UAS-hug and tub-Gal4/TM3 fly lines [26], and heterozygote siblings were used as controls.

For larval rescue experiments, lines used were P(9036)/TM3 (parental line 1), UAS-TeTxLC; P(9036)/TM3 (parental line 2), and hugS3, P(9036)/TM3 (parental line 3). The genotypes assayed were +/UAS-TeTxLC; and P(9036)/P(9036) for control 1 (C1) + hugS3, P(9036)/P(9036) for control 2 (C2) and +/UAS-TeTxLC/+; hugS3, P(9036) for experimental (see Figure 6B). Feeding phenotypes were counted from 100 eggs per collection. Five independent collections per genotype were carried out.

For the adult feeding assay, experimental flies (hug promoter construct driving UAS-TeTxLC expression) and control flies (hugS3-Gal4 flies and UAS-TeTxLC flies crossed with wild-type) derived from 0- to 4-h egg collections were allowed to develop on indicated food for several days. After overnight feeding (for example, on standard fly food or PBS only), flies were allowed to feed on apple juice agar plates containing red-colored food being assayed. At given time points, randomly chosen individuals per genotype were removed and dissected under a dissection microscope. Preparations were fixed and mounted in Mowiol (see below) for microscopic analysis.

Molecular and microarray analyses

The hug construct was made by cloning a 1.5-kb PCR fragment containing the hug regulatory region (amplified from genomic DNA using 5′– CTTCAGGGCCTTGGCTG and 5′– GGGACAACTGATGCCACG as primers) into a pCaSpeR-AUG-Gal4 vector [10]. The direct hug promoter YFP construct was made by replacing AUG-Gal4 of the hug-pCaSpeR construct, with a YFP fragment derived from YFP-pCS2+ vector (Clontech, Palo Alto, California, United States). Transgenic flies were obtained following standard injection protocols.

Microarray experiments were, in principle, done as described previously [45] using Affymetrix (Santa Clara, California, United States) GeneChips representing some 13,500 genes. Egg collections (0–4 h) of the P(9036)/TM3-GFP line were allowed to develop for an additional 22 h at 25 °C. Homozygous P(9036) larvae were hand-picked under a fluorescence microscope, and total RNA was isolated using the NucleoSpin RNA II Kit (Macherey-Nagel, Düren, Germany). GeneChip hybridization and data analysis was done as described [45].

Histochemistry and fluorescence microscopy

Histochemical in situ hybridizations were done following standard protocols, with the slight modification of replacing the proteinase K digest with an overnight incubation of the dissected and fixed larval brains in methanol at −20 °C prior to hybridization. Samples were mounted either in Canada balsam or in Mowiol (12 ml glycerol, 4.8 g Mowiol 40–88, 12 ml H2O, and 24 ml 200 mM Tris [pH 8.5]), and images were taken using a Zeiss (Oberkochen, Germany) LSM 510 META in transmission mode. Fluorescence in situ hybridizations were done using the Tyramide Signal Amplification Kit (PerkinElmer, Wellesley, California, United States) and following the manufacturer's instructions. Overnight incubation with digoxygenin- and/or fluorescein-labeled riboprobes was followed by post-hybridization for additional 2 h. Detection of the first riboprobe with peroxidase (POD)-coupled antibody was performed by overnight incubation at 4 °C, followed by the first staining reaction using fluorescein-tyramide at 1:150 dilution and allowing the reaction to run for 10 min at room temperature. After inactivation of POD by incubation with 10 mM HCl in Drosophila Ringer's solution for 10 min, the second riboprobe was detected by overnight incubation with POD-coupled antibody at 4 °C. The second staining reaction was performed by applying Cy3-tyramide at 1:150 dilution for 10 min at room temperature. In cases of dual marker protein detection, primary antibodies (α–ßGal, Cappel, or α–GFP [Abcam, Cambridge, United Kingdom], used at 1:1,000) were applied together with first POD-antibody and secondary fluorescent antibody (Cy5-coupled α-rabbit, diluted at 1:200 [Jackson Immunoresearch, West Grove, Pennsylvania, United States]) was applied together with second POD-antibody. Samples were mounted in Mowiol and evaluated using a Zeiss LSM 510 META in confocal multitracking mode, generating optical 1- to 1.5-μm sections (using a Zeiss 40×/1.2W C-Apochromat lens) or 2.5-μm sections (using a Zeiss 25×/0.8Imm Plan-Neofluar lens). For direct detection and unmixing of GFP/YFP fluorescence, larval brains of appropriate genotype were dissected in chilled Drosophila Ringer's solution on ice, and mounted without fixation in PBS, using coverslips as spacers and nail polish as sealant. Immediate analysis was performed by emission fingerprinting, using a Zeiss LSM 510 META in confocal lambda mode. Other antibodies used for immunofluorescence were 22C10 diluted 1:100 (Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank, Iowa City, Iowa, United States) and α–elav, diluted 1:300 (Developmental Studies Hybridoma Bank), as well as Alexa488-coupled α-rat and α-mouse antibodies, each diluted 1:200 (Molecular Probes, Eugene, Oregon, United States) and Cy3-coupled α-rat and α-rabbit antibodies diluted 1:200 (Jackson Immunoresearch, West Grove, Pennsylvania, United States). Nuclear counterstaining was performed using Draq5 (Biostatus Ltd., Leicestershire, United Kingdom), diluted 1:1,000 together with secondary antibodies. The GFP antibody co-labeling the corpora allata nuclei (Torrey Pines Biolabs, Houston, Texas, United States) was used at 1:1,000 dilution. The 3D reconstruction of optical sections and figure post-processing were done using Volocity 2.6 (Improvision, Lexington, Massachusetts, United States) and Photoshop 7.0 (Adobe Systems, San Jose, California, United States) on a Mac G4 computer (Apple Computer, Sunnyvale, California, United States).

Supporting Information

Figure S1. Qualitative Graphical Representation of Feeding Analysis

Flies were scored (none, traces, or full) based on amount of red food color in the gut and crop. Overnight treatment was for 12 h. Arrows represent transfer to fresh food, either of the same type or different. For each feeding regimen, two independent experiments were carried out, and for each experimental set and time point, ten individuals were taken out randomly and dissected. The top two graphs are graphical representations of the results depicted in Figure 6D. For experiments with quinine yeast as test food, 12 ± 2 individuals showed displayed phenotypes.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0030305.sg001

(3.1 MB TIF).

Figure S2. Fortuitous Staining of Corpora Allatum

Fortuitous staining of corpora allatum nuclei (shown in green, arrows) by α-GFP antibody from Torrey Pines Biolabs in wild-type larvae (A) and adults (B and C) relative to the neuropile (22C10, shown in red) and the cortex (DNA marker Draq5, shown in blue). We do not know the reason for this, as α-GFP antibodies from other sources do not show this cross-reactivity.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0030305.sg002

(9.7 MB TIF).

Acknowledgments

We thank G. Wahlström, R. F. Stocker, K. Scott, D. Schmucker, L. B. Vosshall, B. Gerber, S. Noselli, T. Klein, C. O'Kane, and T. Siegmund for fly lines and vectors, and T. Kastilan for help with the transgenics. This work was supported by Forschungszentrum Karlsruhe and by grants from DFG (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft) to MJP.

Author Contributions

CM and MJP conceived, designed, and performed the experiments, analyzed the data, contributed reagents/materials/analysis tools, and wrote the paper.

References

  1. 1. Schwartz MW, Woods SC, Porte D, Seeley RJ, Baskin DG (2000) Central nervous system control of food intake. Nature 404: 661–671.
  2. 2. Pflugfelder O (1958) Entwicklungsphysiologie der Insekten. Leipzig (Germany): Akademische Verlagsgesellschaft Geest & Portig. 490 p.
  3. 3. Dethier VG (1976) The hungry fly. Cambridge: (Harvard University Press). 489 p.
  4. 4. Clyne PJ, Warr CG, Freeman MR, Lessing D, Kim J, et al. (1999) A novel family of divergent seven-transmembrane proteins: Candidate odorant receptors in Drosophila. Neuron 22: 327–338.
  5. 5. Gao Q, Chess A (1999) Identification of candidate Drosophila olfactory receptors from genomic DNA sequence. Genomics 60: 31–39.
  6. 6. Vosshall LB, Amrein H, Morozov PS, Rzhetsky A, Axel R (1999) A spatial map of olfactory receptor expression in the Drosophila antenna. Cell 96: 725–736.
  7. 7. Clyne PJ, Warr CG, Carlson JR, et al. (2000) Candidate taste receptors in Drosophila. Science 287: 1830–1834.
  8. 8. Scott K, Brady R, Cravchik A, Morozov P, Rzhetsky A (2001) A chemosensory gene family encoding candidate gustatory and olfactory receptors in Drosophila. Cell 104: 661–673.
  9. 9. Gao Q, Yuan B, Chess A (2000) Convergent projections of Drosophila olfactory neurons to specific glomeruli in the antennal lobe. Nat Neurosci 3: 780–785.
  10. 10. Vosshall LB, Wong AM, Axel R (2000) An olfactory sensory map in the fly brain. Cell 102: 147–159.
  11. 11. Marin EC, Jefferis GS, Komiyama T, Zhu H, Luo L (2002) Representation of the glomerular olfactory map in the Drosophila brain. Cell 109: 243–255.
  12. 12. Wong AM, Wang JW, Axel R (2002) Spatial representation of the glomerular map in the Drosophila protocerebrum. Cell 109: 229–241.
  13. 13. Heisenberg M, Borst A, Wagner S, Byers D (1985) Drosophila mushroom body mutants are deficient in olfactory learning. J Neurogenet 2: 1–30.
  14. 14. Stocker RF, Heimbeck G, Gendre N, de Belle JS (1997) Neuroblast ablation in Drosophila P[GAL4] lines reveals origins of olfactory interneurons. J Neurobiol 32: 443–456.
  15. 15. Stocker RF (1994) The organization of the chemosensory system in Drosophila melanogaster: A review. Cell Tissue Res 275: 3–26.
  16. 16. Stocker RF, Schorderet M (1981) Cobalt filling of sensory projections from internal and external mouthparts in Drosophila. Cell Tissue Res 216: 513–523.
  17. 17. Singh NR (1997) Neurobiology of the gustatory systems in Drosophila and some terrestrial insects. Microsc Res Tech 275: 3–26.
  18. 18. Gendre N, Luer K, Friche S, Grillenzoni N, Ramaekers A, et al. (2004) Integration of complex larval chemosensory organs into the adult nervous system of Drosophila. Development 131: 83–92.
  19. 19. Penzlin H (1985) Stomatogastric nervous system. In: Kerkut GA, Gilbert LI, editors. Comprehensive insect physiology. Oxford: Pergamon Press. pp. 371–406.
  20. 20. Hartenstein V, Tepass U, Gruszynski-Defeo E (1994) Embryonic development of the stomatogastric nervous system in Drosophila. J Comp Neurol 350: 367–381.
  21. 21. Aubele E, Klemm N (1977) Origin, destination and mapping of tritocerebral neurons of locust. Cell Tissue Res 178: 199–219.
  22. 22. Zinke I, Kirchner C, Chao LC, Tetzlaff MT, Pankratz MJ (1999) Suppression of food intake and growth by amino acids in Drosophila: The role of pumpless, a fat body expressed gene with homology to vertebrate glycine cleavage system. Development 126: 5275–5284.
  23. 23. Riddiford L (1993) Hormones and Drosophila development. In: Bate M, Arias AM, editors. The development of Drosophila. Cold Spring Harbor (New York): Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press. pp. 899–939.
  24. 24. Klein T, Campos-Ortega JA (1997) Klumpfuss, a Drosophila gene encoding a member of the EGR family of transcription factors, is involved in bristle and leg development. Development 124: 3123–3134.
  25. 25. Yang X, Bahri S, Klein T, Chia W (1997) Klumpfuss, a putative Drosophila zinc finger transcription factor, acts to differentiate between the identities of two secondary precursor cells within one neuroblast lineage. Genes Dev 11: 1396–1408.
  26. 26. Meng X, Wahlstrom G, Immonen T, Kolmer M, Tirronen M, et al. (2002) The Drosophila hugin gene codes for myostimulatory and ecdysis-modifying neuropeptides. Mech Dev 117: 5–13.
  27. 27. Park Y, Kim YJ, Adams ME (2002) Identification of G protein-coupled receptors for Drosophila PRXamide peptides, CCAP, corazonin, and AKH supports a theory of ligand-receptor co-evolution. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 99: 11423–11428.
  28. 28. Riehle MA, Garczynski SF, Crim JW, Hill CA, Brown MR (2002) Neuropeptides and peptide hormones in Anopheles gambiae. Science 298: 172–175.
  29. 29. Brogiolo W, Stocker H, Ikeya T, Rintelen F, Fernandez R (2001) An evolutionarily conserved function of the Drosophila insulin receptor and insulin-like peptides in growth control. Curr Biol 11: 213–221.
  30. 30. Ikeya T, Galic M, Belawat P, Nairz K, Hafen E (2002) Nutrient-dependent expression of insulin-like peptides from neuroendocrine cells in the CNS contributes to growth regulation in Drosophila. Curr Biol 12: 1293–1300.
  31. 31. Rulifson EJ, Kim SK, Nusse R (2002) Ablation of insulin-producing neurons in flies: Growth and diabetic phenotypes. Science 296: 1118–1120.
  32. 32. Zars T, Fischer M, Schulz R, Heisenberg M (2000) Localization of a short-term memory in Drosophila. Science 288: 672–675.
  33. 33. Estes PS, Ho GL, Narayanan R, Ramaswami M (2000) Synaptic localization and restricted diffusion of a Drosophila neuronal synaptobrevin–green fluorescent protein chimera in vivo. J Neurogenet 13: 233–255.
  34. 34. Siegmund T, Korge G (2001) Innervation of the ring gland of Drosophilamelanogaster. J Comp Neurol 431: 481–491.
  35. 35. Friggi-Grelin F, Coulom H, Meller M, Gomez D, Hirsh J, et al. (2003) Targeted gene expression in Drosophila dopaminergic cells using regulatory sequences from tyrosine hydroxylase. J Neurobiol 54: 618–627.
  36. 36. Schroter U, Menzel R (2003) A new ascending sensory tract to the calyces of the honeybee mushroom body, the subesophageal-calycal tract. J Comp Neurol 465: 168–178.
  37. 37. Connolly JB, Roberts IJ, Armstrong JD, Kaiser K, Forte M, et al. (1996) Associative learning disrupted by impaired Gs signaling in Drosophila mushroom bodies. Science 274: 2104–2107.
  38. 38. Sweeney ST, Broadie K, Keane J, Niemann H, O'Kane CJ (1995) Targeted expression of tetanus toxin light chain in Drosophila specifically eliminates synaptic transmission and causes behavioral defects. Neuron 14: 341–351.
  39. 39. Thorne N, Chromey C, Bray S, Amrein H (2004) Taste perception and coding in Drosophila. Curr Biol 14: 1065–1079.
  40. 40. Wang Z, Singhvi A, Kong P, Scott K (2004) Taste representations in the Drosophila brain. Cell 117: 981–991.
  41. 41. del Campo ML, Miles CI, Schroeder FC, Mueller C, Booker R, et al. (2001) Host recognition by the tobacco hornworm is mediated by a host plant compound. Nature 411: 186–189.
  42. 42. del Campo ML, Miles CI (2003) Chemosensory tuning to a host recognition cue in the facultative specialist larvae of the moth Manduca sexta. J Exp Biol 206: 3979–3990.
  43. 43. Fiala A, Spall T, Diegelmann S, Eisermann B, Sachse S, et al. (2002) Genetically expressed chameleon in Drosophilamelanogaster is used to visualize olfactory information in projection neurons. Curr Biol 12: 1877–1884.
  44. 44. Wang JW, Wong AM, Flores J, Vosshall LB, Axel R (2003) Two-photon calcium imaging reveals an odor-evoked map of activity in the fly brain. Cell 112: 271–282.
  45. 45. Zinke I, Schutz CS, Katzenberger JD, Bauer M, Pankratz MJ (2002) Nutrient control of gene expression in Drosophila: Microarray analysis of starvation and sugar-dependent response. Embo J 21: 6162–6173.